Huffington Magazine Issue 69 | Page 69

Exit “negative news is more retweeted than positive news.” But nonnewsy tweets, such as social updates, were shared more when they were positive. The authors advised people seeking out more followers to “sweet talk your friends or serve bad news to the public.” Those who maintain that cheerful trumps dreadful online frequently cite the research of Wharton School professor Jonah Berger, the author of “Contagion: Why Things Catch On.” In one study, Berger and his co-author Katherine Milkman analyzed nearly 7,000 stories that made it to The New York Times’ most-emailed list to figure out if they could decode a pattern to the articles’ popularity. They found uplifting stories (i.e. “Wide-Eyed New Arrivals Falling in Love with the City”) were more viral than depressing ones. But “highly arousing content,” like articles that induced anxiety or anger, did best of all. “Online content that evoked high-arousal emotions was more viral, regardless of whether those emotions were of a positive (i.e. awe) or negative (i.e. anger or anxiety) nature,” the researchers noted — a conclusion echoed by a slew of other studies. When emotionally charged content gets readers agitat- TECH HUFFINGTON 10.06.13 ed, their instinct is to hit “share.” These findings have major implications for our experience online, far beyond how to win more followers. They suggest that social media can actually reward — through its currency of shares, retweets and “likes” — outbursts of rage and anything that make us agitated. Hype wins, nuance loses. The problem with the viral nature of extreme emotions is that Social media can actually reward... outbursts of rage and anything that make us agitated. we both ingest that content and emulate it. If that’s what we share then that’s what we’ll see, which in turn will shape how we act. It’s not a leap to suggest heated emotions breed more heated emotions online, or rage more rage. A study by Facebook’s data science team found that if people used negative words, such as “petty” or “lame,” in their status updates, their friends became more likely to include negative words in their own posts. The bump in usage persisted even three days after the initial post, and the effect also applied to positive terms.