The Missouri Reader Vol. 37, Issue 2 | Page 9

interpersonal communication skills (BICS) more quickly because social language is survival language. BICS are acquired through informal nonacademic interactions, encompass the language of the playground and cafeteria, and involve social negotiations using facial expressions, gestures and body language. Students will gain conversational proficiency within two to four years and may appear to be fluent speakers of the second language or L2 (Cummins, 1981). Younger English Learners will often speak English without an accent, thus prompting teachers to believe the child has achieved fluency in L2. However, if the child has obtained only a conversational level of proficiency and not an academic level of proficiency, he/she will have difficulty with cognitively complex concepts in L2. Cognitive academic language proficiency (CALP) is the ability to use conceptual knowledge as a tool for learning across content areas (Cummins, 1981). Academic language is cognitively demanding. As teachers we ask students to do a myriad of complex tasks: to compare and contrast, define, categorize, connect, explain, apply, analyze, synthesis, and evaluate (Reiss, 2012). Consequently, it can take ELs five to seven years to fully acquire academic language proficiency in L2. Therefore, it is important that instructional support for the development of cognitive academic language proficiency in English extend well beyond conversational skills or basic English. According to Thomas and Collier (1995), the amount of formal schooling a student has received in his/her first language (L1) will significantly impact how long it will take a student to learn a second language (L2). The theory of the Common Underlying Proficiency (Cummins, 1984) maintains that academic knowledge and skills learned in L1 will transfer to L2. For example, if a child knows how to read in his/her first language, the child understands the basic concepts of reading such as decoding and making connections to text. The various academic skills students have learned in L1 such as defining, predicting, analyzing, interpreting metaphors, problem solving, and evaluating can be applied as they learn and study in L2. Krashen (1982) developed the input hypothesis as a means for understanding the process of second language acquisition. An EL will better comprehend new information when the input is one step above his/her current competency level. Krashen refers to this type of input as comprehensible input; it is the zone between actual and potential language development. For example, if the student is competent at level i, then input at i + 1 is the most comprehensible for learning new information. While similar to Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development (1978), Krashen (1985) applied his concept only to second language acquisition and concentrated on how to make language input understandable by using: (a) appropriate speech, (b) appropriate non-verbal cues to support language use, (c) teaching techniques to clarify content concepts, and (d) clear explanations of academic tasks. Herrera and Murry (2011) offer the following applications of i + 1 comprehensible input:  Previewing key content vocabulary in the first and second languages.  Highlighting or outlining key concepts.  Gesturing to emphasize key points.  Providing time for clarification of key concepts in the first language with a peer or paraprofessional.  Simplifying and modifying the academic language used during instruction. (p. 300) English Learners thrive in classrooms containing language-rich literacy instruction (Herrera, Perez, & Escamilla, 2010; Ogle & Correa-Kovtun, 2010; Reiss, 2012; Peregoy & Boyle, 2013; Shanahan & Beck, 2006). Students need a wide range of reading materials available at their instructional or independent reading level (Allington, 2007). Oral language development must be an integral part of ELs literacy instruction, thus requiring extensive opportunities to talk and use new vocabulary and concepts (Echevarria, Vogt, & Short, 2004). Prior knowledge should be activated and ©The Missouri Reader, 37 (2) p.9