The Missouri Reader Vol. 37, Issue 2 | Page 22

strengths, and scaffold their language learning burdens. Understanding sentence structure. Meaning is carried by the grammar of the language, so students need to be able to understand and use sentence structures to express their meanings. This is a major difference between ELLs and EO. ELLs need for explicit attention to grammatical markers in a sentence. For example, words such as “even though” or “nevertheless” can be confusing to ELLs. They both signal that the sentence which follows them offers some contradictory meaning. Students need to learn the meaning of such words and how they are used to signal contradictions in the following sentences. This work can be integrated into guided reading discussions. Emphasis on writing academic sounding texts can be achieved by comparing and contrasting lexical, structural, and discourse features of conversational English with more formal academic texts. An example of successful student work is helpful. Also, students need to be able to connect various language structures and syntax with specific academic discourses such as comparing and contrasting, or cause and effect. Gaining meaning from academic language, such as “compared to, different from, and due to”, for example, is one skill which is important. Students also need to be able to use syntax to understand, articulate and write expository prose (Dutro & Helman, 2009, Zwiers, 2008). Learning to self-edit as suggested above is on-going. (Lenski & Verbruggen, 2010). Asking students to examine their writing for verb tense problems, for example, should be a persistent step in final editing practice. Nevertheless, throughout their learning trajectories, EL learners’ language will be marked with grammatical and phonological errors which are signs of what they have acquired and what they still need to learn. More intentional vocabulary instruction. Roessingh and Elgie (2009) found that by grade 4, ELLs’ reading comprehension can be noticeably behind their classmates because underlying concept development has not kept pace in their first language. The authors cite numerous studies which show that ELL vocabulary outcomes are sometimes as many as three years behind EO student outcomes. The vocabulary gap of these learners is often masked because ELLs can pick up oral language relatively easily if they arrived here at an early age, and because early reading texts have a tightly controlled vocabulary, the significance of the vocabulary lag is not detected early. Thus, by grade 4, early-arrived ELLs have to use concepts that they are learning through a language which they are still learning. So, reading becomes harder for them. If they only speak English at school, it is understandable that there is a gap. As a result, reading teachers need to augment their vocabulary study and harness technology to help ELLs increase their vocabulary. Understanding and using vocabulary is a constant need for ELLs. A familiar strategy for teaching ELLs is to begin with oral language so that learners can hear a word multiple times. Zimmerman (1997) asserts that learners need 8 to 10 instances of hearing a word for them to master the phonology and meaning of a word; moreover, ELLs need to use the word in a conversation (Zwiers & Crawford, 2009). Hu and Nation (2000) reported that “a second language reader needs to know 98% of the words in a text in order to be able to read it unassisted and to effectively guess the meaning of unknown words from context”(p.403). This was verified by Nation (2006) who found that “a vocabulary base of 8000-9000 word families is typically necessary for comprehension of written text such as a novel or newspaper” (p.59). This huge task for second language learners is compounded by the fact that word knowledge is something that needs to unfold over time. Beck, McKeown, and Kucan’s (2002) vocabulary framework is widely used to decide which words to teach ELLs. Their three tiers of words are: tier 1 of common occurring words, tier 3 are words which are low frequency words associated with special knowledge domains. ©The Missouri Reader, 37 (2) p.22