The Missouri Reader Vol. 36, Issue 1 | Page 21

program of supplying pizzas to students who read a certain number of books has all three of these characteristics. First, pizzas are very attractive rewards to students. Second, the pizzas are given only when the required amount of books are read with no consideration given to the types of books read, how carefully the books were read, if the students were able to gain new knowledge from the reading, etc. Third, the rewards of the program (the pizzas) are artificially tied to reading the books – there is no natural link between reading and pizzas. The same analysis can be made for the extrinsic motivators of ice cream parties and for the promise of the principal kissing a pig – both are attractive rewards to students, both are not contingent upon students meeting a true literacy goal (other than perhaps having a paper signed by a parent that ―proves‖ the student read for a certain number of minutes), and both have no natural link to reading. Gallagher (2003) noted that practices such as these suggest that reading is such an unpleasant task that rewards (or bribes) are necessary. Further, this type of extrinsic motivation can produce self-terminating behavior. After children receive an external motivator (such as a pizza), their reading often ceases. This can lead to students becoming dependent upon rewards and recognition to complete reading activities. Students are trained to believe that reading should be done only if there is some reward to be received at the completion of it. In addition, the rewards will need to continually increase in attractiveness as the recipients become no longer motivated. For example, the principal kissing a pig will be a captivating event the first time, but will lose its effectiveness if it is repeated over and over again. As the reward gradually loses its appeal, the provider of the reward will have to find something more attractive to replace it. This cycle will continue as long as an extrinsic motivator is necessary for the behavior to occur. Guthrie & Wigfield (2000) noted the paradoxical effect that rewards and incentives have on motivation. Rewards can increase shortterm attention on activities, but students who are in an environment where extrinsic rewards are dominant will become more extrinsically motivated. They will become more focused on high grades, correct answers, and task completion and less committed to comprehending text, improving reading skills, or enjoying reading. These students frequently rely on memorization and guessing. Further, they avoid challenging tasks and give up easily when frustrated. Eliminating extrinsic motivators from school classrooms is neither necessary nor realistic. However, when teachers use extrinsic rewards to motivate their students to read, they should do so in a way that does not undermine students‘ intrinsic motivation. Brophy (2004) noted that the key to effectively rewarding students is to do so in a manner that supports students‘ motivation to learn and does not encourage them to conclude that they engage in an activity only to earn rewards. For example, rather than rewarding a student with a pizza for reading a certain number of books (which may be below the student‘s reading level), teachers may reward the student for reading and comprehending a book at his/her reading level with extra free reading time in class or a gift certificate to a local bookstore. If rewards are used appropriately and effectively, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation will complement each other (Covington & Mueller, 2001). ©The Missouri Reader, 36 (1) p. 21