Social Democrats Louth Issue 1 Volume 1 | Page 29

and with no conscious corrective of unconscious bias—the gap between grades given to working-class Hannah and to middle-class Hannah increased. None of the participants was apparently aware that their ‘objective’ opinions were skewed by unconscious assumptions about class and relative intelligence. It’s more than possible that they would no longer consciously agree with those assumptions. They no doubt believed that their judgements were unbiased. These results demonstrate why, for instance, teachers should not be asked to grade their own students for certification. They also demonstrate the impossibility of a claim to class-blindness, gender-blindness, colour-blindness, etc. on an individual, group, or national level. Centuries of cultural biases—the ‘truths’ that ‘everybody knew’—don’t disappear because we know better now. They just hang out in our collective unconscious waiting for their moment in the light. They remain invisible except by their effects, and even then, only if we’re brave and honest enough to examine them. In our own image The second study, Pygmalion in the Classroom, demonstrates one powerful negative consequence of those unexamined assumptions by showing the extent to which teachers’ expectations influence a student’s achievements. Robert Rosenthal and Lenore Jacobson told teachers at a primary school that they were administering a test to identify ‘academic bloomers’, i.e. those who were expected to ‘enter a period of intense intellectual development over the following year’. After the test, the researchers reported that the results showed that specific students would be ‘bloomers’. In reality, the test was a regular IQ test, and the ‘bloomers’ were randomly selected. These students were not in any way distinguished by their test performances. Nevertheless, a year later when the researchers returned to the school and administered the same test, the results were ‘astonishing’, as Adam Alter said in Psychology Today: the ‘bloomers,’ who were no different from their peers a year ago, ‘now outperformed their unselected peers by 10-15 IQ points’. The researchers concluded that the teachers, who didn’t realise that they had been misled, and, under the impression that they were ‘harvesting unseen talent’, expected more from these students. Those expectations produced ‘a self-fulfilling prophecy in which the students who were baselessly expected to bloom actually outperformed their peers’. The importance of these two studies is enormous. Not only are working-class students assumed to be markedly less intelligent than their middle-class counterparts, even when they give identical performances, but teachers’ expectations have a profound effect on student performance. Students, that is, live up or down to exp