Re: Spring 2014 | Page 86

Leaders of the pack In the 80s we had Barbara Woodhouse and Train Dogs the Woodhouse Way. In this show Barbara would check, choke and shout at dogs for disobeying and ‘making fun’ of their owners. Barbara’s popular dog training show was replaced in later years with Caesar Milan arguably, a more charismatic pack leader - with the whitest teeth you have ever seen. Caesar also checks, chokes and shouts at dogs, this time for being ‘dominant’ and trying to challenge their human pack leader. Given the similarity of these two TV training methods, one might think that not much has changed in the last 30 years when it comes to dog training. But it has, and a training revolution is on the cards... Now for the science bit... To explain how far we have come and the science behind the ‘force-free’ animal training movement, we have to go right back to the 1890s and an American psychologist called Edward Lee Thorndike (‘Teddy’ to his friends). Teddy was fascinated by animals and he wanted to find out whether animals could learn by observation. He set up ‘puzzle boxes’ which had weighted buttons or levers that opened doors. He placed animals inside (mostly cats) and once the doors were opened, allowed the animals to run free. He also rewarded them with food. He let some animals watch other animals set themselves free but found this made no significant difference in how long it took to achieve this. What he did learn however, was that with every trial, the animals got quicker at using the buttons or levers and setting themselves free. This paved the way for 84 training as Teddy’s cats in boxes taught us The Law of Effect. The Law of Effect predicts that if an association is followed by a “satisfying state of affairs” then it is strengthened (so if we reward a specific behaviour the animals does it more, faster). If an association is followed by an “annoying state of affairs” it is weakened (if we punish an animal for a specific behaviour they don’t do it as much). at different schedules of reinforcement (how often we reinforced specific behaviours) and how that affected learning and showed us that stopping reinforcement or punishment will also stop previously learnt behaviour (if a dog learns that when he sits he gets a biscuit - if you remove the biscuit, after a while a dog will stop sitting). He called this extinction. And so came Ivan Pavlov who went on to teach us that you could actually cue behaviour with objects and sounds as long as they predicted rewarding or punishing outcomes: i.e. that if a bell always meant food was given to a group of dogs, that the bell (even without the food) caused the dogs to salivate. The bell was a secondary reinforcer. Of course the dogs salivated for the food but they learnt to salivate for something previously meaningless - a bell! This was really useful but we only really used the punishment part within the ‘dominance’ dog training model. That was because trainers and behaviorists felt that nearly all the behaviour dogs showed was based upon the undesirable ambition to become higher than their master in the pack. We therefore wanted to reduce this undesirable behaviour and so partook in punishments as we understand that punishment decreased behaviour. ‘And there is every dog trainer’s best friend, B.F. Skinner. Skinner furthered our understanding by teaching us the effect that different types of reinforcement had on behaviour. He made a quadrant of four ways to increase or decrease specific behaviour: Positive Punishments (something horrible is added like an electric shock); Positive Reinforcement (something brilliant is added like a mars bar); Negative Reinforcement (something horrid being taken way like someone holding you under water then letting you up for air); and Negative Punishment (something brilliant being taken away like the removal of your favorite top out of your wardrobe). He found all four of these reinforced affected future behaviour outcomes. He also looked However, meanwhile in the late 80s there were some people doing things a little differently. A new training method was devised that worked solely on marking any correct behaviour with a predictor of food (like Pavlov’s bell), and then giving the animal a reward. And so it was that clicker training was born. Clicker training was a method of training popularised by Karen Pryor in the late 80s in her book Don’t Shoot the Dog. Initially used to train marine life to do tricks in parks like Sea World, the method uses a little metal box to mark