Latest Issue of the MindBrainEd Think Tank + (ISSN 2434-1002) 3 MindBrained Bulletin Think Tank Work Mem Mar 1 2 | Page 6

to improve the working memory capacity of my students, encouraging them to stay on-task seems much more aligned to the pedagogical aim of creating engaging materials at the right level of challenge. Of course, this will still favour students with high working memory capacity, who, if I’ve been successful in my material creation, will now be super-focused and extract maximum information, and thus show greater learning gains (and, in fact, Peter himself has published research suggesting high working memory capacity is correlated with increased learning from multimedia sources). Two, this view of working memory capacity suggests that exceeding capacity is not just about giving students too many things to remember at once, but also giving them low-priority activities at the same time as high-priority ones. Sometimes one signal needs as close as it can get to 100% attention. For example, if, as we are giving instructions, we also ask our students to pass around a handout, we are increasing the noise around the intended signal. Whereas if, as we give instructions, we also display them in writing (say via slides on a TV or projector), we reinforce the signal. This also increases the duration of this signal, allowing low working memory capacity students to pay repeated attention to it, as needed. And three, the need to guide attention towards the signal and away from noise provides another reason why students benefit from knowing what the aim of the activity is before they start trying to perform it. If they know in advance which features of the activity they need to attend to, it must surely be easier for them to use and produce task-relevant information. My second question is much more of a speculation—or an area for future research! Working memory is also associated with long-term memory. One way of viewing working memory is as the connection between attention and long-term memory, or prior knowledge. Prior knowledge can be divided into semantic and autobiographical memory, which are distinct yet interacting knowledge representations. The semantic component seems to help structure episodic memories by contributing gist knowledge (for example, a few days after teaching a class, you might not be able to remember whether an absent student was actually present or not, as although they were absent on that one occasion, your gist memory of them being part of the class on seven previous occasions in the last month interferes with the episodic memory of that day). Your extant knowledge guides what you attend to in your immediate environment, and this interaction determines learning. One way of viewing working memory is as the connection between attention and long-term memory, or prior knowledge. 5