Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 51-5 | Page 33

Memory rehabilitation post-stroke ing (6). Findings are discrepant from previous CCT studies in which improvement in objective memory has been described (34–36). Possible explanations for inconsistency in findings may relate to discrepancies in training platforms, samples and design of these studies. Most previous CCT studies have used CogMed QM training, which selectively targets WM. In addition, previous studies have commonly used mixed aetio- logy samples (35, 36), which limits generalization to stroke. Moreover, the most widely cited stroke CCT study (34) failed to incorporate follow-up assessment, an active control or blinded assessors. Limitations are particularly pertinent given the vulnerability of CCT to placebo effects (6) and may account for disparate findings. Given that Lumosity TM is arguably the most popular and widely recognized CCT programme currently available (30) and purports to remediate everyday memory, its effectiveness requires ongoing empirical validation. There were short-term effects of MSG intervention on subjective ratings of everyday and memory failures and CCT close other ratings of PM difficulties, though these were not seen at follow-up. Within this context, it is possible that the inclusion of “booster sessions” to consolidate treatment gains may be necessary to main- tain the benefits of the MSG intervention over time; a finding previously described in brain-injured samples (37). Regarding self-reported strategy use, while all participants reported a significant increase in strategy use over time, improvement was only maintained for participants allocated to the MSG. This interaction was not seen for external strategy use. These findings suggest that MSG, but not CCT, may be effective in improving the frequency of internal strategy use; a finding supported by recent qualitative analysis (38). Group discrepancies in strategy use may relate to the context in which this information is acquired. While participants in the MSG were taught strategies in a structured group format and actively encouraged to apply them to everyday tasks, participants in the CCT intervention appeared to spontaneously implement internal strategies to improve their task performance. This adoption of strategies in CCT has been previously described, and has been suggested as contributing to the transfer of computerized interventions to untrained activities (39). Nevertheless, the likely variable manner in which participants implemented these strategies to everyday tasks may account for their lack of sustained effectiveness and everyday applicability. Findings are consistent with qualitative feedback from CCT parti- cipants who considered strategy use as “cheating” and failed to see the generalization to real-world strategy use (38). Future research should seek to explore the efficacy of combined CCT with compensatory memory 349 strategy training to encourage generalization of spon- taneously implemented strategy use. Some methodological limitations of this study are acknowledged. Importantly, the study compared group, centre-based training of compensatory strate- gies with individual, home-based computer training. Consequently, we were unable to control for the ef- fects of group socialization not present in CCT. Thus, we cannot rule out the possibility that some of the improvement evident in MSG participants may have resulted from group participation, rather than group content per se. However, this was a deliberate deci- sion on behalf of researchers to maintain ecological validity through delivery of the interventions as they are clinically intended, to facilitate the translation of findings. Similarly, although the MSG is a manualized intervention, participants allocated to this condition may have had the opportunity to raise specific goals, not seen in CCT participants. Future research should seek to explore the impact of group delivered CCT to assist in delineating the influence of content vs medium of delivery. The subjective nature of several outcome measures may be prone to measurement error. Regular interac- tion with researchers for intervention, but not WC participants may also have led to Hawthorne effects, particularly given the subjective nature of outcomes (40). However, these effects do not explain differences between MSG and CCT groups, which remains the aim of this paper. Similarly, participants in the study often had difficulty specifying memory goals, parti- cularly given their abstract nature. Notwithstanding, participants appeared to understand the process of goal setting, and potential difficulties were controlled for across groups. Interestingly, the majority of WC participants described achieving a memory goal in the absence of a structured intervention. Results highlight the potential therapeutic effect of goal setting but also support the added benefit of MSG training beyond these benefits. Findings further demonstrate the need for a control condition to robustly explore the impact of health interventions. In addition, eligibility was not limited to individuals with objective memory impairment, which may explain the relatively mild nature of memory impairment in this sample. The inclusion criterion of subjective memory complaints was selected deliberately to reflect the patient population that is most likely to present for me- mory rehabilitation services. Access to memory rehabi- litation services is not usually contingent on objective memory impairment from a clinical perspective. Our primary aim was to understand if these interventions improved everyday memory function irrespective of the underlying cause of change. Nonetheless, findings J Rehabil Med 51, 2019