Internet Learning Volume 3, Number 1, Spring 2014 | Page 98

Internet Learning research to have the highest scientific impact score among U.S. and Canadian social psychologists (Nosek et al., 2010). The Theory of Planned Behavior model is shown in Figure 1 (Ajzen, 2013a). The key tenets of the model include direct and indirect measures of attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control that are used to predict intention, which subsequently predicts behavior (Ajzen, 1991, 2012, 2013a). According to this expectancy-value model, which weights beliefs about actions by their value, behavior is the actual manifestation of an individual’s action in a particular circumstance. Intention is the proximal predictor of a person’s behavior and indicates an individual’s willingness/readiness to demonstrate that behavior. The model postulates a strong relationship between intention and behavior for those behaviors that are under one’s own volitional choice. Intention is predicted from an individual’s attitude toward the behavior, subjective norms regarding the behavior, and perceived behavioral control over the behavior. Attitudes are defined as “the individual’s positive or negative evaluation of performing the behavior” and are assessed by having individuals respond to bipolar adjective scales regarding the behavior under examination (e.g., good–bad; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980, p. 6). Subjective norms are “the person’s perception of the social pressures put on him to perform or not perform the behavior in question” (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980, p. 6). This social pressure is measured as a general sense of what important others think one should do. Finally, perceived behavioral control is one’s sense of his or her ability to perform the behavior under examination, which can be assessed by having individuals rate the extent that performing the behavior is up to them and whether they perceive they have control over it. Underlying these attitudes, norms, and control perceptions are one’s beliefs which are weighted by the subjective value of these beliefs. Specifically, behavioral beliefs are the accessible thoughts a person holds regarding a certain behavior. These beliefs are tempered by one’s evaluations of the outcomes associated with these beliefs. In terms of the model, each of a person’s behavioral beliefs is multiplied by the outcome evaluation associated with that belief. Then, each of these products is summed to form an indirect assessment of one’s attitude toward the behavior. In a similar manner, normative beliefs are the salient expectations perceived by individuals that are set by members of a relevant referent group (e.g., family members, co-workers). These beliefs are weighted by one’s motivation to comply with these expectations, and the sum of products (i.e., each belief multiplied by the motivation to comply with it) constitutes an indirect measure of one’s subjective norms regarding the behavior. Finally, control beliefs are thoughts regarding factors in the setting which may either impede or enhance the performance of the behavior. These factors are weighted by the power each control factor holds over the individual. One’s level of perceived behavioral control is the sum of products (i.e., each control factor multiplied by its power of control) to perform the behavior. Given the wide application of the theory, several notable meta-analyses speak to the model’s utility in predicting intentions to engage in a multitude of behaviors. For example, in a review of 185 independent and varied applications of the model conducted prior to 1997, Armitage and Conner (2001) found that across all behaviors studied, the correlation of intention and perceived behavioral control was significant, with perceived behavioral control accounting for 27% of the variance 97