Harvard International Review | Page 29

WORLD IN REVIEW Western Theory, Global World Western Bias in International Theory staff writer ALEX YOUNG S cholars of international relations often operate under the assumption that their project is to generate the truth, to come to some objective understanding of what the international sphere is and how it works. Most contemporary international relations theory, though, is tainted by a major source of bias: it is produced in western nations by western authors for western readers. International relations theory is skewed westward, which impairs its ability to explain and to produce social good. Much of this western bias is due to the historical political and military dominance of the west; history is written by the victors, and philosophy seems to be, too. Amitav Acharya and Barry Buzan argue in their 2010 book Non-Western International Relations Theory: Perspectives On and Beyond Asia that the vast majority of schools of thought in international relations are outgrowths of one western philosophical tradition or another: realism comes from the work of Thucydides, in their research; this means that western researchers use almost exclusively western subjects. The body of knowledge generated by studies in game theory and behavioral economics, then, is considered to reveal certain universal truths about human beings, whereas in reality it is highly culturally specific. In 1995, then-UCLA graduate student in anthropology Joe Henrich traveled to Peru to study the economic behavior of indigenous peoples. Henrich found that the subjects of his research behaved very differently in game theoretic simulations than North American subjects did. North Americans are generally eager to propose and to reward fair distributionsof resources in games; Peruvian natives, on the other hand, did not seem to care nearly as much about fairness. This is not to say that some cultural, ethnic, or regional groups played the game more or less rationally than others; no style of play is necessarily better or worse. Instead, Henrich’s findings simply point to a diversity of “There is nothing inherently biased about applied game theory, but as it is practiced in modern academic circles, the field privileges westerners over people of other backgrounds.” Hobbes, and Machiavelli; liberalism derives from Kant, Locke, Smith, and others; Marx and Engels were German; and even those constructivist and postmodern accounts of international relations that emphasize relativism and diversity draw mostly on the ideas of French authors such as Pierre Bordieu and Michel Foucault. Of course, non-westerners from Sun Tzu to Amartya Sen have also made valuable contributions to political science and international relations theory, but on the whole, western voices have dominated and continue to dominate international relations discourse. The problem does not seem likely to go away, either. The field of international relations is becoming increasingly grounded in economics and psychology. The growing use of applied game theory to explain and predict phenomena in international affairs means that international relations theory has become entangled with the assumptions, ideological commitments, and empirical findings of behavioral economics—a body of knowledge that exhibits a western slant. There is nothing inherently biased about applied game theory, but as it is practiced in modern academic circles, the field privileges westerners over people of other backgrounds. Social psychologists use convenient subjects values across cultures that translates into different goals and strategies. Henrich replicated this study in various countries and cultures around the world, observing a wide range of behaviors across cultures. These findings challenge the common, western position that game theoretic study reveals fundamental features of human nature. His work points to one of the sources of bias in international relations theory: it is built on research in behavioral economics and psychology that commonly assumes that people operate the same way regardless of culture and that takes mainly westerners as the subjects of its study. The concept of human nature that informs international re-lations theory is a western one, produced through studies of western subjects but erroneously applied to people the world over. Despite these limitations, applied game theory was widely employed in prominent publications to explain Vladimir Putin’s actions and intentions after the Russian invasion of Crimea last spring. Moreover, international relations theory is largely the product of western thinkers at western institutions. In 2011, the Christian Science Monitor ranked the top 25 graduate programs in international relations worldwide; 19 were in the US, five were in the UK, and one was in Canada. Summer 2014 • H A R V A R D I N T E R N A T I O N A L R E V I E W 29