Forensics Journal - Stevenson University 2012 | Page 11

FORENSICS JOURNAL Experts continued hypothesizing on the correlation between a social violation (such as a cruel and pointless murder) and its physical correlation (such as a generally recognizable flaw in the brain). Now couched in simplistic terms such as “nature or nurture,” this dispute underlies the enduring debates about the normal and abnormal. (See Churchland, 1998; Joseph, 2001; Fischman, 2011; Lindsay, 2012) Chapter 8, “The Body Speaks,” provides a compelling visit into the minds and practices of Lacassagne and his assistants. Once having displaced human speech with the body’s own signs, the investigative teams formed a radically new concept of the corpse. After a person’s demise, the body is still host to all sorts of activity. Its temperature, muscular tissue and skin deterioration all change gradually but at different rates. Lacassagne thus wanted to find a corpse quickly in order to pinpoint the hour of death. If a corpse was discovered six months after reported missing, investigators were left to register the gradations of flies and their larvae that permeated the corpse in order to estimate the time of death. Asphyxiation was a common cause of death in Lacassagne’s time, so he had to train himself and his students to be able how to distinguish an accidental drowning from purposeful drowning or willful strangulation. Parenthetically, early forensics tested their hypotheses by placing—and killing—dogs in analogous situations. Today researchers continue to look for the source of criminal predispositions in the circuitry of the brain. While Lacassagne would have cheerfully embraced the contemporary attention—through science and public fascination—with the violent tendencies of clever criminals, he also would likely assent to Starr’s closing remarks. Namely, in trying to find the source for a human monster, we invariably encounter an abyss. REFERENCES THE (AB)NORMAL BRAIN Churchland, P. (1998). Toward a Cognitive Neurobiology of the Moral Virtues. Topoi, 17, 83-86. Retrieved from http://www.cogsci. ucsd.edu/~rik/courses/readings/PMChurchland98-neuroMorals.pdf After describing how the individual paths of Lacassagne and Vacher eventually converged—and the legal and social circumstances of that encounter—Starr concludes with a report on a professional and scientific dispute that still lingers. Namely, how can we explain an anomaly such as the human monster? Or, at least, what should the proper response be when an otherwise seemingly normal human commits a monstrous act? Fischman, J. (2011, June 12). Criminal Minds. The Chronicle Review. B6-B8. Foucault, M. (2003). Abnormal: Lectures at the Collège de France, 1974-1975. New York: Picador. Looking for an etiological anchor to criminal acts has always been a central task for legalists, criminologists, politicians, moralists and sociobiologists. Their findings, if accepted by authorities, could be used for an array of responses from the proper punishment of the miscreant to correcting or preventing the predisposition for committing the misdeed. Upon Vacher’s death his brain was dissected with sections sent to six different experts. To envision these specialists walking home with a section of a serial murderer’s brain offers a moment of grim humor—what did they dare hope to find? Joseph, J. (2001). Is Crime in the Genes? A Critical Review of Twin and Adoption Studies of Criminality and Antisocial Behavior. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 22(2), 179-218. Lindsay, R. (2012, December/January). The Debate Over Enhancements. Free Inquiry, 32(1), 18-21. Retrieved from http://www.secularhumanism.org/index.php?section=fi&page=lindsay_intro_32_1 Starr, D. (2010). The Killer of Little Shepherds: A true crime story and the birth of forensic science. New York: Knopf. They quibbled over which sorts of lesions or damaged tissues on Vacher’s brain were possibly relevant. One expert, focusing on the section that supposedly controlled speech, asserted this section of Vacher’s brain was so formidable that he had the potential for public oratory equal to any current politician or leader.1 Here Lacassagne and his adherents began challenging the popular expertise of criminologist Cesar Lombroso, who contended that the source of most social harm could be found in “the born criminal.” 1 Tiryakian, Edward. (1986). Hegemonic Schools. Structures of Knowing. Ed. Richard Monk. University Press of America *Many thanks to students in INDSC, Spring, 2012—Law/Crime—for their thoughtful comments and suggestions. The controversy over Vacher’s brain extended from arcane findings in professional meetings to lively newspaper accounts. One columnist mocked the exchanges with a sarcastic dialogue among imaginary experts, one of whom claims that it is true Vacher disemboweled some innocent shepherds, but analysis of his brain shows that “he would not have been capable of doing any harm to a fly sitting on the head of one of those shepherds.” This marks an eerie parallel to the movie Psycho, in which Norman Bates, after slaughtering his mother among others, muses to himself how he could have done such thing when he would not even harm the fly resting upon his nose. 9