Family & Life Magazine Issue 14 | Page 21

sometime between the ages of threeand five-years-old. those synapses can be recruited for something different”. In the early years of our lives, our brains are rapidly and actively developing, forming new synapses – the connection between neurons – and crucially, pruning them later on. There is a steady rise of synaptic connections up until the age of three, then starts declining after you blow out the five candles on your birthday cake. In layman’s terms, your brain finally decides the connections that it needs and chooses to retain, and removing the connections that are not as important. Unfortunately, childhood memories belong to the latter group. Other studies also point to the hippocampus being a bit of a forgetful chap in its younger years due to the immense growth of nerve cells in that small area of the brain when the child is growing up. As more and more new memories are created, the immature hippocampus loses track of the older memories. It’s rather ironic that the organ responsible for memory creation and storage forgets where it stored the memory. Computational neurologist Paul King explains: “Young children can learn very quickly, and since learning is basically a rearrangement of the representational structures of memory, this learning likely has a side effect of degrading memories while the brain seeks its optimal representational strategy. Unlike a computer, the brain does not have empty memory locations waiting to be filled.” Instead, the brain engages in a process called shredding, constantly removing and forming new synapses and neurons, including the ones involved in remembering. We can lose our memories if the synapses that connect to these neurons are not used. According to psychologist Patricia Bauer, “if you never use the memory, Language, and the learning of it, is also widely agreed to play a big role in infantile amnesia. The brain thrives on structure and since the foundation of language – grammar, etc. – has a conceptual structure, it provides the brain with a support framework for knowledge which consequently helps to stabilise memory. Simply put, a young child is unable to describe an event because he or she doesn’t have the requisite language skills to describe it. It probably explains why I couldn’t remember my soiled diaper shenanigans; when my vocabulary expanded, I had upgraded to the portable potty. THE ROLE OF CULTURE AND EMOTION However, this still didn’t explain my fragmented memories of sleeping on my father’s lap during the long journeys on public transport. Psychologist Carole Peterson has an explanation – if a childhood memory was more emotional and dense (the child understands the who, what, when, where and why), the memory would be five times more likely to be retained as opposed to inconsequential fragments. Peterson and her colleagues from the Memorial University of Newfoundland have also managed to pinpoint the age that childhood memories begin to vanish through a series of studies. First, they assembled a group of children between the ages of four and 13 to describe their three earliest memories. The children’s parents stood by and verified that these memories did indeed occur and was not just a figment of the children’s imaginations. Then, two years later, the children were interviewed again to see if they still remembered the memories that they had shared. More than a third of those aged 10 and older still remembered the memories they had shared during the first interview. However, the younger children, especially those who were first interviewed when they were four, had largely forgotten the memories they had shared, even when they were prompted. “They said: ‘No, that never happened to me.’ We were watching childhood amnesia in action,” says Peterson. Interestingly, another study conducted in 2009 by Peterson alongside two compatriots, Qi Wang from Cornell and Yubo Hou from Peking University, suggests that the ability to retain childhood memories is also dependent on cultu