ElmCore Journal of Educational Psychology October, 2014 | Page 51

Science-Fellows® situations; they serve to identify the problem and connect it with experiences from the past. There is, however, no action directly related to metacomponents, they simply direct what actions will follow. Performance components are the actions taken in the completion of a problem-solving task. Performance components go beyond metacomponents in that they perform the function also of weighing the merit and or consequences of actions in comparison to other options rather than simply identifying options. Sternberg’s third proposed type of intelligence is the knowledge-acquisition component. This type is characterized by the ability to learn new information in order to solve a potential problem. This type is much more abstract and may or may not be directly related to a current problem-solving task (Driscoll, 2001). This three-leveled view of intelligence comprises the componential aspect of Sternberg’s theory, but this is only one of three parts to his larger triarchic theory of intelligence (Kearsley, 2001c). Sternberg’s (1988) theory adds the components of feedback to theories of cognitive development; this suggests that an individual’s social interaction has some impact on cognitive development. In fact, one of the three parts of his theory is based on the context in which learning takes place; this subpart of the theory “specifies that intelligent behavior is defined by the sociocultural context in which it takes place and involves adaptation to the environment, selection of better environments, and shaping of the present environment” (Kearsley, 2001c). The addition of social context as a factor in cognitive development links Sternberg to the interactional theories of development of Bruner (1977, 1986) and Vygotsky (1978). These theories, and others of this type, are premised on the assumption that learning does not occur in a vacuum. Therefore, one must discuss the social and cultural contexts of learning. Driscoll (2001) says, “Of central importance is viewing education as more than curriculum and instructional strategies. Rather, one must consider the broader context in how culture shapes the mind and provides the toolkit by which individuals construct worlds and their conceptions of themselves and their powers” (p. 221). Once one understands how information is stored in memory and the developmental process of learning, the question that naturally arises is how one can best understand a student’s developmental progress and what he or she knows. It is important to address domain-specific knowledge and processing capacities as well as capacities that are non-domain specific. Dietel, Herman, and Knuth (1991) provide some important guidelines regarding assessment and evaluation. One of the most important points is that data gathered during the assessment process, which in turn, will be used for evaluation purposes, is guided by one’s beliefs in regard to learning. As one can surmise from the review of literature on information processing and memory, this can be a very complex task. They report that “From today’s cognitive perspective, meaningful learning is reflective, constructive, and self-regulated. People are not seen as mere recorders of factual information but as creators of their own unique knowledge structures” (p. 3). How, then, accurate assessments can be made becomes troublesome. Assessment and Evaluation Concerns One might think that a traditional area of strength for the educational system has been the assessment of knowledge and cognitive skills. However, as previously discussed, the cognitive taxonomy of educational objectives developed by Bloom et al. (1956) and revised by Anderson and Krathwohl (2000) show there is a significant difference between lower- and higher-level thinking and knowing. Unfortunately, the testing process now used in the United States overemphasizes lower-level knowing (Striggins, 2002) The fact that standardized test scores seem to dictate most educational practice suggests a direct conflict of interest for ensuring that students are taught and assessed in higher-level cognitive skills. Striggins argues that the failure to balance classroom assessment of higher-level skills with standardized assessments has drastically hurt the educational system. More recently, “most of the national curriculum standards expect teachers to create active learning environments that stimulate higherlevel student thinking” (Freiberg, 2002, p. 56). In view ElmCore® Journal of Educational Psychology (01) 1001