ASEBL Journal Volume 10, Number 1 | Page 50

ASEBL Journal – Volume 10 Issue 1, January 2014 which, through the process of natural selection, would have led to a “’debilitation of aggressive responses’” and a “’strengthening of inhibitory controls’” (167-168, quoting himself from Hierarchy in the Forest). What Boehm is describing here is the evolution of the conscience, which is like a “social mirror” highlighting our behavioral accounts, good and bad, for us to view in full (172). Without addressing brain science or consciousness fully, this is where individual differences come into play – how one can use this very social mirror in a calculating manner to subtly deceive while appearing good. Even Adam Smith in the eighteenth century (his notion of the impartial spectator) recognized individual differences in the competition between caring and personal gain (though as a product of his time Smith chalks up such differences to class). At any rate, Boehm admits that since the tendency to altruism is slight, the hunter-gatherer groups he examines prove that “cultural support” is necessary and apparent if the group is going to survive cooperatively and without serious conflicts (273). For instance, in discussing tit-for-tat, Boehm says that the exchange of goods is less important than the “spirit of generosity” such exchange produces (302). Granted, but one knows that if he boosts the generous spirit of the group he stands a better chance of gain, for without any likelihood of (eventual) profit a player is sure to defect. For those interested in evolutionary studies (especially humanists interested in ethics), Boehm’s work is crucial in that it takes complex questions of morality out of a theoretical cloud and places them squarely in the human arena (of altruism and shame). Boehm’s scholarly research of prehistory and anthropological work in contemporary people give credence to our innate sense of fairness and capacity for reciprocity. We evolved away from the hierarchical model to the egalitarian. More precisely, Boehm is able to delineate how and why human conscience arose: more than the function of the individual in a group and more to the function of the group on the individual. While using the imperfect geologic record we have (of human remains, evidence of human culture, climate shifts affecting our prehistory) to complete the puzzle about the origins of morality, Boehm’s book makes a significant contribution to this important discussion. - Gregory F. Tague ▬ Mark Pagel. Wired for Culture: Origins of the Human Social Mind. NY: W.W. Norton, 2012. 432 pgs. $29.95US Hardcover. ISBN: 978-0393065879 Mark Pagel’s Wired for Culture is an eloquent and erudite examination of (to borrow from Richard G. Klein) the human career. While Pagel focuses on the universal aspects of culture (“knowledge, beliefs, and practices” [2]), much of the discussion hovers around the individual related to cooperation and moral behavior, the human tendency to form and adhere to small groups. Pagel places the blossoming of culture at around 80,000 years ago, by which time we not only learned from imitation but moreover began to innovate and re-engineer what we had learned. We then passed that understanding on to succeeding generations so that (via an intellectually ratcheting-up effect) symbolic artifacts (such as jewelry, paintings, and carvings) began to appear. In this way the bits of culture, from an idea to a technological feature, would “act like” a gene in terms of transmission and reproduction among 50